caspi p-factor nihms523372

Telechargé par craftworx
The p Factor: One General Psychopathology Factor in the
Structure of Psychiatric Disorders?
Avshalom Caspi1,2,3,4, Renate M. Houts1, Daniel W. Belsky5, Sidra J. Goldman-Mellor6,
HonaLee Harrington1, Salomon Israel1, Madeline H. Meier1, Sandhya Ramrakha7, Idan
Shalev1, Richie Poulton7, and Terrie E. Moffitt1,2,3,4
1Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University
2Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University
3Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center
4Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College
London
5Center for the Study of Aging and Human Development, Duke University Medical Center
6Center for Developmental Science, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
7Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department, of Preventive
and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago
Abstract
Mental disorders traditionally have been viewed as distinct, episodic, and categorical conditions.
This view has been challenged by evidence that many disorders are sequentially comorbid,
recurrent/chronic, and exist on a continuum. Using the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and
Development Study, we examined the structure of psychopathology, taking into account
dimensionality, persistence, co-occurrence, and sequential comorbidity of mental disorders across
20 years, from adolescence to midlife. Psychiatric disorders were initially explained by three
higher-order factors (Internalizing, Externalizing, and Thought Disorder) but explained even better
with one General Psychopathology dimension. We have called this dimension the p factor because
© The Author(s) 2013
Corresponding Author: Terrie E. Moffitt, Duke University, Box 104410, 2020, W. Main St., Ste. 201, Durham, NC 27708,
Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Author Contributions
A. Caspi, R. M. Houts, and T. E. Moffitt created the study concept and design and drafted the manuscript. A. Caspi, S. Ramrakha, R.
Poulton, and T. E. Moffitt acquired the data. A. Caspi, R. M. Houts, D. W. Belsky, S. J. Goldman-Mellor, H. Harrington, S. Israel, M.
H. Meier, I. Shalev, R. Poulton, and T. E. Moffitt analyzed and interpreted the data. D. W. Belsky, S. J. Goldman-Mellor, H.
Harrington, S. Israel, M. H. Meier, I. Shalev, and R. Poulton critically revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. R. M.
Houts and H. Harrington provided statistical analysis. A. Caspi, R. Poulton, and T. E. Moffitt obtained funding. H. Harrington and S.
Ramrakha provided administrative, technical, and material support. A. Caspi and T. E. Moffitt supervised the study.
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information may be found at http://cpx.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data
NIH Public Access
Author Manuscript
Clin Psychol Sci
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 March 01.
Published in final edited form as:
Clin Psychol Sci
. 2014 March ; 2(2): 119–137. doi:10.1177/2167702613497473.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
it conceptually parallels a familiar dimension in psychological science: the g factor of general
intelligence. Higher p scores are associated with more life impairment, greater familiality, worse
developmental histories, and more compromised early-life brain function. The p factor explains
why it is challenging to find causes, consequences, biomarkers, and treatments with specificity to
individual mental disorders. Transdiagnostic approaches may improve research.
Keywords
psychiatric epidemiology; developmental psychopathology;
DSM
A psychiatric nosology—the classification of mental disorders—is a practical tool. A
nosology is useful for research because it is used to integrate and guide empirical studies. A
nosology is useful for health-care delivery because it is used to make prognoses and to
decide on treatment need and choice of treatment. The
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders
(5th ed.;
DSM-5
; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) is the current
ascendant nosology in clinical psychology and psychiatry. It may not be perfect (Sanislow et
al., 2010), but it is what many of us work with in both research and clinical practice (Kupfer,
Kuhl, & Regier, 2013).
A persistent challenge to the
DSM
and related nosologies is comorbidity, the coexistence of
two or more conditions or disorders (Hasin & Kilcoyne, 2012; Kessler, Chiu, Demler,
Merikangas, & Walters, 2005) Comorbidity rates are very high in psychiatry and conform
roughly to the rule of 50%: Half of individuals who meet diagnostic criteria for one disorder
meet diagnostic criteria for a second disorder at the same time, half of individuals with two
disorders meet criteria for a third disorder, and so forth (Newman, Moffitt, Caspi, & Silva,
1998). The high rates of comorbidity observed among mental disorders suggest that there
may be a more parsimonious structure to psychopathology than implied by current
nosologies that identify many separate and distinct disorders. This article begins by
providing a brief historical review of empirical research on the structure of psychiatric
disorders. It then offers an empirical update, suggesting that most common psychiatric
disorders are unified by a single psychopathology dimension representing lesser-to-greater
severity of psychopathology that is associated with compromised brain integrity.
Soon after the publication of the
DSM-IV
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994),
psychological scientists noted the need for research that would examine patterns of
comorbidity to “elucidate the broad, higher-order structure of phenotypic psychopathology”
(Clark, Watson. & Reynolds, 1995, p. 131) We responded to this call by using confirmatory
factor analysis (CFA) to evaluate alternative hypotheses about the latent structure underlying
10 common mental disorders among young adults, ages 18 to 21 years (Krueger, Caspi,
Moffitt, & Silva, 1998). In contrast to the prominence of categorical models in the
classification of adult psychopathologies, dimensional models had long enjoyed success in
research on the classification of childhood psychopathologies, and empirical studies had
converged on two primary dimensions as a way to characterize childhood disorders:
Internalizing (including anxious and depression symptoms) and Externalizing (including
aggressive, delinquent, and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms; Achenbach & Edelbrock,
Caspi et al. Page 2
Clin Psychol Sci
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 March 01.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
1981). We could see no reason why this highly replicable two-dimensional structure of
psychopathologies should suddenly vanish when research participants and patients suddenly
turned age 18 years. Our data confirmed that a two-factor model accounted for the
comorbidity of different young-adult disorders, and it bore a striking similarity to the model
of childhood psychopathologies.
On the basis of this initial finding, we put forth the hypothesis that common
DSM
psychiatric disorders in adulthood may be characterized by two underlying core
psychopathological processes: an Internalizing dimension indicating liability to experience
mood and anxiety disorders, such as major depression (MDE), generalized anxiety disorder
(GAD), panic disorder, and social phobia; and an Externalizing dimension indicating
liability to experience substance disorders and antisocial disorders. During the past 15 years,
multiple studies in different parts of the world, in different age-groups, in general
community samples, and in clinical populations (e.g., Forbush & Watson, 2013; Kendler,
Prescott, Myers, & Neale, 2003; Krueger, 1999; Slade & Watson, 2006; Vollebergh et al.,
2001) have replicated this basic finding (Krueger & Markon, 2006, 2011).
With the publication of the
DSM-5
and debate fomenting over the need for a dimensional
nosology (Insel, 2013), now is a good time to take stock of what is known about the
structure of psychopathology. We have drawn on insights stemming from six recent findings
about the epidemiology of mental disorders.
First, life-course epidemiology points to the need for longitudinal research designs to study
the course of psychopathology. Previous research on the structure of psychopathology has
been carried out using cross-sectional designs, focusing on individuals who report symptoms
within a specified period (most often using the past 12 months as the reporting period).
However, research has shown that cross-sectional snapshots mix single-episode, one-off
cases with recurrent and chronic cases, which are known to differ in the extent of their
comorbid conditions, the severity of their conditions, and possibly the etiology of their
conditions. This is true for a variety of common disorders, including, for example,
depression and alcohol-use disorders (Jackson & Sartor, in press; Monroe & Harkness,
2011), but also for psychotic experiences (van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul, &
Krabbendam, 2009). That is, whether manifested as recurrence or chronicity, some people
are more prone than others to have persistent (as well as comorbid and severe)
psychopathology, These results underscore the need to take the longitudinal course of
mental disorders into account when modeling the higher-order structure of psychopathology.
Second, sequential comorbidity points to the need to model multiple disorders over time.
Previous research has focused on comorbidity as defined by the co-occurrence of two or
more disorders at the same time, but both retrospective (Kessler et al., 2011) and
prospective-longitudinal (Copeland, Shanahan, Costello, & Angold, 2011) research has
shown that comorbidity is also sequential. For example, longitudinal research has shown
that GAD and MDE are linked to each other sequentially such that each disorder increases
the likelihood of developing the other disorder in the future among individuals who
presented with only one condition at one point in time (Moffitt et al., 2007). These results
Caspi et al. Page 3
Clin Psychol Sci
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 March 01.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
underscore the need to take into account both concurrent and sequential comorbidity when
evaluating the structure of psychopathology.
Third, psychotic disorders can be included in models of the structure of psychopathology. In
most previous studies, researchers have omitted psychotic disorders from their evaluation of
the structure of psychopathology. There are practical explanations for this omission (e.g.,
most surveys of psychiatric disorders do not assess psychotic symptoms), but their absence
from studies of the structure of psychopathology is conspicuous for three reasons: (a) New
research on the dimensional model of psychosis has directed attention to the fact that
psychotic symptoms are more commonly experienced in the general population than
previously assumed, (b) psychotic disorders are striking in their especially high rates of
comorbidity, and (c) psychotic disorders have extraordinary high economic burden, as
expressed in the number of years lost due to ill health, disability, or early death (Murray et
al., 2012; van Os et al., 2009). A few researchers recently have incorporated psychotic
symptoms and symptoms of schizotypal personality disorders into their assessment of the
structure of psychopathology, pointing to the existence of a third, distinct Thought Disorder
spectrum (Kotov, Chang, et al., 2011; Kotov, Ruggero, et al., 2011). These results
underscore the concern that efforts to model the structure of psychopathology without
consideration of psychotic symptoms may not capture the true structure in the population.
Fourth, twin studies and risk-factor studies have suggested not only that there are substantial
phenotypic correlations among pairs of psychiatric disorders but also that the liability to
many disorder pairs (e.g., schizophrenia and bipolar disorder; MDE and GAD; and alcohol
and cannabis dependence) is influenced by the same genetic factors (Kendler, 1996;
Lichtenstein et al., 2009; Sartor et al., 2010) and that many disorder pairs are characterized
by shared intermediate phenotypes (Nolen-Hoeksema & Watkins, 2011). These findings
imply that the causes of different disorders may be similar, highlighting the potential value
of a transdiagnostic approach to psychiatric disorders. The value of a transdiagnostic
approach has been further underscored by evidence that different disorders often respond to
the same treatments (Barlow et al., 2011).
Fifth, symptom variation above and below diagnostic cut points implies modeling disorder
data at the level of symptom scales. Researchers in most previous studies of the structure of
psychopathology have modeled
DSM
disorders as dichotomous variables, although the few
that have used symptom scales have generated comparable results (e.g., Markon, 2010).
Diagnostic thresholds increasingly have been acknowledged to be somewhat arbitrary, and it
has been recognized that there is meaningful and useful clinical information both above and
below diagnostic thresholds (Kessler et al., 2003; Lewinsohn, Shankman, Gau, & Klein,
2004). The
DSM-5
also opted to emphasize dimensional conceptualizations of psychiatric
disorders.
Sixth, the possibility of one General Psychopathology factor should be tested. This issue has
arisen from the observation that disorders are positively correlated not just at the disorder
level but substantially so at the spectrum level as well; for example, the correlation between
the Externalizing and Internalizing spectra is ~.5. and the correlation between the
Internalizing and Thought Disorder spectra has been estimated at ~.6 (Wright et al., 2013).
Caspi et al. Page 4
Clin Psychol Sci
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 March 01.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Given high correlations at the spectrum level, Lahey et al. (2012) suggested the intriguing
possibility that in addition to propensities to specific forms of psychopathology (e.g.,
Internalizing vs. Externalizing), there may be one underlying factor that summarizes
individuals’ propensity to develop any and all forms of common psychopathologies. They
used confirmatory factor models to test a hierarchical bifactor model that derives a general
factor from the correlation matrix between different mental disorders and found that
depression, anxiety, substance use, and conduct/antisocial disorders all loaded strongly on a
single factor, in addition to specific Internalizing and Externalizing spectra (Lahey et al. did
not include symptoms of psychosis in their work).
A useful way to think about the meaning of such a general factor in psychopathology is by
analogy in relation to cognitive abilities. These abilities are dissociable into separate
abilities, such as verbal skills, visuospatial skills, working memory, or processing speed.
Nonetheless, the general factor in intelligence (called the g factor) summarizes the
observation that individuals who do well on one type of cognitive test tend to do well on all
other types of cognitive tests (Deary, 2001; Jensen, 1998; Spearman, 1904). Although
specific factors account for variation in each test, the g factor accounts for the positive
correlation among all test scores, suggesting that all cognitive functions, to some extent, are
influenced by common etiology. Just as there is a general factor of cognitive ability, it is
possible that there also is a general factor of psychopathology.
Given the aforementioned new findings and insights, we used data from a comprehensive
prospective-longitudinal study of mental disorders; during the past 20 years, we repeatedly
assessed symptoms of 11 kinds of common adult mental disorders in a representative birth
cohort, from ages 18 to 38 years. The research reported here had four aims. First, we tested
alternative models of the structure of psychopathology using data that take into account
information about the dimensionality, persistence, co-occurrence, and sequential
comorbidity of mental disorders, including psychosis. Second, we evaluated the validity of
the structure of psychopathology by testing associations between the factors obtained and
independent information about the study members’ personality functioning and life
impairment. Third, we tested the family histories and developmental histories associated
with each of the factors representing the structure of psychopathology. Fourth, we tested the
hypothesis that individual differences in severe and impairing psychopathology are
associated with compromised brain integrity from early life.
Method
Dunedin Study
Participants are members of the Dunedin Muitidisciplinary Health and Development Study,
a longitudinal investigation of health and behavior in a complete birth cohort. Study
members (
N
= 1,037; 91% of eligible births; 52% male, 48% female) were all individuals
born between April 1972 and March 1973 in Dunedin, New Zealand, who were eligible for
the longitudinal study based on residence in the province at age 3 and who participated in
the first follow-up assessment at age 3. The cohort represents the full range of
socioeconomic status in the general population of New Zealand’s South Island and is
primarily White. Assessments were carried out at ages 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 18, 21, 26, 32,
Caspi et al. Page 5
Clin Psychol Sci
. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 March 01.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
1 / 29 100%
La catégorie de ce document est-elle correcte?
Merci pour votre participation!

Faire une suggestion

Avez-vous trouvé des erreurs dans linterface ou les textes ? Ou savez-vous comment améliorer linterface utilisateur de StudyLib ? Nhésitez pas à envoyer vos suggestions. Cest très important pour nous !